FYI: Autistic Women and Autistic Writers Exist, and They Might Even Be Modified by Adjectives Such As “Successful” Rather Than “Egocentric” or “Mindblind”

I’ve been going through old computer files lately, and I realized that I never posted the paper I read at the Feminism(s) and Rhetoric(s) conference that took place at Michigan State this past October (the week before our protest of the Autism Speaks walk, actually). So, here it is — my essay.

—-

“Melanie,” she writes, and I imagine her doing so in an armchair, a red velvet armchair, this woman enunciating each syllable of my name, if only to make sure I comprehend her—“I hope as we go forward, Melanie, I hope you come to understand that at many levels what does and does not apply to you”—I stop reading, grind my teeth, poke my tongue in a developing cavity, if only to make my wince more wince-worthy—and continue on with her letter. “It’s not meant to personally challenge you,” she blathers, “but are the observations and ways of those with very different life experiences. Other people have different life experiences than you, Melanie, but I understand how difficult it is for you to put yourself in others’ shoes.”

I stop reading. It is difficult for me to fit into others’ shoes. My feet are incredibly narrow size nines, and I often fall out of my shoes—my shoes. And then there was toddlerhood, me walking so feverishly and insistently on tiptoes, my mother recalls, that the doctors considered cerebral palsy! (with an exclamation point) and hurriedly put my legs in casts below the knees, then braces, only to find out that it wasn’t cerebral palsy, that it wasn’t a symptom of anything with a legally recognized name, at least not anything legally recognized in the U.S. until 1995, at least not a symptom of anything other than Melanie being Melanie and what the hell is wrong with Melanie? There are empaths, and then there are dis-empaths—and as a teenager I was pegged into that escapably inescapable designation, that of the autism spectrum disorder, the one that, if you believe the charities, creeps into your child’s room at night and steals her soul, steals her ability to walk flat-footed, steals her ability, as the blathering woman in the imaginary red velvet armchair put it, to recognize that “other people have different life experiences.”

So much of my childhood was a search for an explanation—a search carried out by my parents, pastors, teachers, counselors, and the elementary school kids who liked to beat me up at recess. One day it’s selective mutism, and the next day it’s all my mother’s fault. One day it’s “let’s get a CATSCAN and make sure she doesn’t have a brain tumor,” and the next day my guidance counselor asks if my father has ever touched me. (And me, being ever the literal-minded autistic, says “yes”—is it illegal for fathers to touch their kids?) Once the Asperger’s autism designation descended from the diagnostic heavens, my capacity to empathize was suddenly eaten up by malfunctioning neurons. My capacity to engage in social relations or maintain eye contact vaporized alongside my personality. My capacity to have capacity was called into question. All these discourses, all these incapacities. Discourse about autism, I think, is far more virulent than autism.

In fact, discourse about autism has reached critical mass. Media outlets harp about the so-called epidemic, likening autism to a fate worse than pediatric AIDS, cancer, and diabetes combined. As of this past week, the autism rate has changed from 1 out of 150 people to 1% of the total population—1 out of 91. Not only this, but autism is said to affect mostly boys, the new statistics reflecting an incidence of autism in 1 out of 58 boys. These days, when I read and hear the numbers, when freshmen at my university tell the campus newspaper that these numbers are “so alarming,” alarming enough for them to fear procreation—I think to Lennard Davis’ work on disability and normalcy, specifically, when he describes the entire field of statistics as eugenics. Davis notes, “Statistics is bound up with eugenics because the central insight of statistics is the idea that a population can be normed. An important consequence of the idea of the norm is that it divides the total population into standard and non-standard subpopulations. The next step in conceiving of the population as norm and non-norm is for the state to attempt to norm the nonstandard—the aim of eugenics” (6).

When I am a number—a gendered number at that, and I mean gendered number both literally and figuratively, because I’ve synaesthetically thought of numbers as being gendered since I was a kid—but… when I am a number, I’m a number to be avoided. A number meant to instill fear and alarm. A number meant to warn parents that I could happen to them. A number that signals the dissolution of marriages and other gratuitous disability-induced horrors. A number that borrows its soundtrack from that classic, repeated knife-stab move in slasher flicks. I can see and feel the numbers as eugenics—all too visually, all too tangibly.

But the fraughtness of autism discourse neither starts nor ends with numbers—it involves our very conceptions of autism and its overlaps with gender, involves that tired misconception of autism precluding empathy, emotion, and personhood. Kidnapper imagery abounds in PSAs and billboards; popular nonprofits mourn the loss of the children that never were. And as reprehensible as these mass-mediated representations are, perhaps more concerning to me (out of my own autism-induced self-centeredness?) (I pose that question snarkily) are the professional discourses that affect me, us, you, them—any and all of us who hold some connection to the amorphous numbers. For as much as we’d like to dismiss the autism-as-thief trope as the next of the myths du jour, such myths find their realities in the various professional discourses that surround autism and the numbers and the gender and empathy issues. In medical discourse, autism is disempathy. It is, as psychologist Simon Baron-Cohen notes, a case of the “extreme male brain” (3). According to Baron-Cohen, autistic people are logicians and systematizers—characteristics in supposed contrast to femininity and empathy and social skills. Autistic neurology is so phallic as to penetrate unsuspecting female minds and make male any and every idiosyncrasy. Scholars in the mood for retro-diagnosis take delight in postulating Emily Dickinson was autistic, or that idiosyncratic fictional characters such as Jane Eyre were autistic. While certainly the autism rate remains higher for boys at a 4 to 1 ratio, the key characteristic for all autistics, per Baron-Cohen, is neurological maleness—such that autistic girls and women become doubly disabled: first by a merciless soul-stealer, and then by a chronic gender-reassigner.

I think to all the ways in which I am distinctly feminine, or distinctly unfeminine—or, conversely, more than a matter of mere is, the ways in which my supposed unfemininity is constructed as such, is rendered a symptom of my supposedly more-male-than-female neurology. The letter from the imaginary-armchair woman—the letter where she claims that I lack understanding of experiences outside my own, of minds outside my own. Or the first time I went to a school dance, where I went up to the DJ and requested the Electric Light Orchestra, to which he replied that he didn’t have any Electric Light Orchestra; so then I began requesting bands who sounded like the Electric Light Orchestra, such as Kansas or the Moody Blues, or Jefferson Starship or Styx—and I kept reciting band after band after band until he yelled at me, over the sounds of 90s grunge, to get the hell out of his face. Or, at this dance, when I grew stiff to the touch, to any touch, and while my female classmates discussed boys and shampoo tips and kitten posters, I wanted to talk about how many top-40 songs ELO had in a four-year period, or recite the list of all of their songs in alphabetical order, or rehearse the band members’ birthdates.

But, as alien as I may seem to describe myself, as rhetorically unaware as my sixth-grade self may seem—I’d posit that the disempathy here, this rhetorical construction of the autist as disempath, is ableist. That is, any assumption about lack of audience awareness by default makes an ableist assumption about who an audience comprises—an audience filled with non-autistic people, or parents, or professionals, or statisticians? Are autistic people considered to lack such capacity that they cannot form and function as their own audience?

Perhaps my delving into rhetoric, writing, and the troubling of audience seems a stark shift in tone here. But as a writer and an autistic and a woman, it doesn’t seem this way to me. The transition seems so natural, not stark, and I’m not even sure that I need a transition, that I need to create some turn-around phrase or some three-point thesis statement that outlines the whole of what I’m saying, what I’m writing. And for this, a compositionist who likes all things neon and 80s might pull out her copy of Linda Flower and start droning on about reader-based prose and cognitive immaturity, what Ann Jurecic referred to as egocentricity in her 2007 article in College English, called “Neurodiversity.” Jurecic’s piece is what I’d label the trademark autism piece in the field of rhetoric and composition, at least, it’s certainly the most well-known, is one of the first if not the first, and it’s so normatively organized, with lots of transitions and other so-called readerly cues. As Ann Jurecic labels autistic writer and scientist Temple Grandin as mindblind, I can’t help but wonder about my own signposting or lack thereof in the essay I read now, how things make so much sense to me but may very well make no sense to you, as if my words here float and crumble, a style begging for an analogy to my strained ways of making and maintaining eye contact. I think to Jurecic’s comparison of Grandin’s edited and published book versus Grandin’s unpublished essays on her web site—a comparison Jurecic uses to argue that Grandin very much lacks audience awareness, that any semblance of organization is likely attributable to heavy editing on someone else’s part. Jurecic writes, “Grandin, of course, is not a college writer; she is a professor whose job requires her to write frequently and well. Her writing is ‘autistic’ in large part because, even after she has written six books and dozens of articles, she still cannot consistently define a line of argument, guide a reader from one point to the next, or supply background for references that will otherwise be unclear” (429).

I should here note that I haven’t hired anyone to do heavy editing on this essay—as much as I may have needed it. I haven’t run this essay by an advisor or committee member. I haven’t visited the writing center, nor have I discussed potential revision strategies with a disability services counselor. I’ve only shared it with the mirror, reading off words in my own eye contact-less, male-but-not-really-male-brained way.

What strikes me about Jurecic is her reliance on Baron-Cohen, who has also researched and written quite prolifically about autism and mindblindness—that is, the supposed inability to imagine the mental states of others. Despite autism’s postulated male, logical influence, she describes the essaying of autistic writers as having an “unfamiliar logic that is challenging to follow” (43). She also notes of autistic writers that, “Clearly, an inability or limited ability to theorize other minds, as with egocentrism or limited empathy, would make communication a challenge” (426). And here I stop and revisit an earlier point, perhaps in my own desperate attempt to mimic good essay conventions, perhaps in my own frenzied manner of transitioning from point A to point Q. Such a stance, that of grounding autistic ways of knowing and expressing in terms of unfamiliarity, inability, challenges, mindblindness, disempathy, limitations, and other items mired in deficit—such a stance leads me to think that some of these autism PSAs need to be revised, to inform parents that autism steals a person’s ability, as I here unfairly quote Jurecic, to “define a line of argument, guide a reader from one point to the next, or supply background for references that will otherwise be unclear” (430). It scares me that scholars and peers in my field have taken a Baron-Cohen turn. It scares me that my peers and professors and students might perceive my ways of knowing, being, and expressing as misfiring neurons, as disempathetic illogicalities. Such deficit-laden rhetoric makes little to no room for theories of neurological difference, makes no room for disability studies, where societal barriers are more disabling than any form of bodily difference.

As Susan Wendell writes, “We need a feminist theory of disability…Disability is not a biological given; like gender, it is socially constructed from a biological reality. Our culture idealizes the body and demands that we control it” (260). I need only think to Tony Atwood to see the relevance of Wendell and other disability theorists—not to mention feminist theorists—to grasp how unquestioned matters of biology go in matters of disability because, as Simi Linton notes, disability is so often conceived of as that “atypical experience of deficit and loss” (5). Attwood, arguably the most well-known Asperger’s specialist in the world, has recently taken to writing about the under-diagnosis of autism in girls and women. He describes such girls as being able to “pass” more fluidly in day-to-day life because they possess certain positive, womanly qualities—unlike the stereotyped representations of the screaming, aggressive autistic boy, autistic girls are more likely to be quiet and “passive” (3). Attwood also contends that neurologically typical girls are more likely to be “maternal” and take autistic girls under their wings and help with social skills (5).

What I find most pertinent about Attwood to this discussion, however, is his embrace of Baron-Cohen’s concept of the extreme male brain. While discussing how autistic individuals have obsessions, or what he terms “special interests,” Attwood maintains that most autistic girls have “typical” girl interests such as kittens or unicorns—but the unrelenting intensity and rigidity of their interests (that is, the detached and weirdly logical male expression of their interests) is what sets them apart (5). A “typical” girl submits her dolls to mock social situations such as dating or going to the mall. An autistic girl lines her dolls up in alphabetical order, or by height or type, and sits in her room for seven hours while observing the flaws and curves of Barbie’s plastic figure.

Where to go with all this—this assumption that autistic people are inherently lacking something, this assumption that autistic women are somehow less than women because of their neurological wiring, this assumption that autistic writers lack audience awareness, when, in reality, autistic people are excluded from most every audience one could even think of, so what practice would we have anyway in imagining the mental states of others when everyone else so wrongly presumes to know our own mental states?

I think to autistic writer Jane Meyerding, who identifies her autistic self as genderless. She writes, “My intellect makes me a feminist. But my gut, my feelings, my self-awareness remain stubbornly and radically un-gendered—at least in the terms of the culture that surrounds me” (157). And: “When people perceive me as aloof, they are sensing an absence of emotional availability. It’s unwomanly of me, in traditional terms, to be the way I am. In feminist terms, it’s un-sisterly. I just have to accept that, for this autistic, it’s normal” (169).

I’d like to think that feminist approaches to disability—that is, any approach that considers social and cultural constructions of difference, rather than neurological imperatives—would not render the autistic woman as un-sisterly or unwomanly. I’d like to think that I could call myself autistic and not be considered unempathetic or mindblind, as lacking in some core feminine trait (as if there exists a checklist of core feminine traits). I’d like not to get letters from mothers of autistic children that patronize me and my approach to the world, and I’d like not to think of such mothers as occupying red velvet armchairs, because the kitschy image of red velvet alone makes me want to gag. I’d like to think that autism organizations at my university and in my city wouldn’t present autistic individuals as lacking humanity, as having a condition that has taken something intrinsic away. I’d like to think that, as my title suggests, autistic women and autistic writers not only exist in space and time, but also exist in categories that are not centered around deficit, loss, and mystery.

7 thoughts on “FYI: Autistic Women and Autistic Writers Exist, and They Might Even Be Modified by Adjectives Such As “Successful” Rather Than “Egocentric” or “Mindblind””

  1. Wonderful essay. I’ve also read that piece by Meyerding, “Growing Up Genderless,” and written about it — I’d had a lot of experiences similar to hers, but rather than perceive myself as agendered I came to see myself as female but masculine.

    As to Tony Attwood’s prediction that typically developing girls might take an autistic girl under their wing and teach her to fit in, I have to say I never experienced this. I never got the infamous “Mean Girls” treatment, either, though; most girls just ignored me, as I ignored them.

  2. Also, some totally insubstantive, stream-of-consciousness responses:

    It is difficult for me to fit into others’ shoes. My feet are incredibly narrow size nines…

    Oh my gosh we might be shoe twins!! I have rather narrow feet (not super-narrow, such that I need Narrow-width shoes, but narrow enough that normal shoes can be loose on me) and my shoe size varies between eight and a half and ten.

    …oh, look, we’re talking about shoez. Clearly we must be Real Women after all!

    [T]he next day my guidance counselor asks if my father has ever touched me. (And me, being ever the literal-minded autistic, says “yes”…)

    Hah, I might well have answered that way, too, if anybody had ever asked me that question. I am also hugely literal-minded; I was not accepted into the gifted program in my school district even though I aced the written test because I refused to speak to the teacher conducting the interview portion. I suspect I did this because I thought I shouldn’t talk to strangers.

    I’m a number meant to be avoided. A number meant to instill fear and alarm. A number meant to warn parents that I could happen to them.

    Yep. I actually had a friend (yes, a friend — our relationship progressed beyond the point I’m about to describe) who felt a kind of superstitious fear of me, fear that I had been sent into his life as a kind of symbol of the Divine Retribution to come, in the form of profoundly disabled children. (This friend was of a deeply religious and superstitious bent, and his sister had once caught him making fun of another kid with disabilities, and told him God would punish him by making all of his children disabled. That he retained this into young adulthood, and that it affected him so deeply as to make him deeply conflicted about our friendship, astonishes me. And not in a good way).

  3. Finally, one nitpick: in the first paragraph, when you’re describing the lady in the armchair writing a letter to you, she should be enunciating every syllable of your name rather than annunciating it. I think. To enunciate is to pronounce distinctly; to annunciate (I think??) is to proclaim loudly.

  4. As a (mostly – I am antisocial but I get by) neurotypical woman, I think you write much more coherently and with more structure than I usually do. I practically never spell out a line of argument – that sort of emerges as I go along, since I am terribly anxious about stating my purpose and argument directly at the start, like they always tell you to in high school and college (what with the “thesis sentence” and the one-justification-per-paragraph and so on). Having read plenty of school essays from NT people, I think the ability to “define a line of argument, guide a reader from one point to the next, or supply background for references that will otherwise be unclear” is a rather rare trait. Most of us have a tendency to ramble and suck with transition sentences. Well, most of us are not very good writers.

    Our gendered division of male and female personality traits and activities always sets off a red flag in me. These expectations do not seem to me at all respectful of individual choice when they tell us we are abnormal for our natural preferences. Can we not embrace a gender identity without conforming to gender norms? I never played with dolls, I was too busy building forts and playing soccer; I loved horses (only thoroughbreds, and some Arabians) but not unicorns, I sewed dresses for their color and texture but never played dress-up and never cared for fashion. To this day I have no idea how to console someone, or tell them what they really want to hear (“you’re not fat at all!”). Yet I believe I am very much a woman, and quite happy with that, though discontented at the assumptions made of us. Perhaps it the narrowness of our norms that is the problem, not the people who cannot fit into them?

  5. Great essay. This really does a good job explaining the problems with the “male brain” idea. (I will not call it a theory.) I’m sure I’ll be using this essay for future reference.

    That Jureric article is an ableist mess. I’d read it before, but reread it after reading your essay, and it definitely falls into the category of Ableist Academic Articles Which Piss Me Off. The assumptions and condescension are just so blatant. Her assumptions that Grandin didn’t “really” write what she actually wrote are infuriating, and remind me of the way slave narratives were produced and received in antebellum America. Narratives such as Harriet Jacobs’ “Incidents in the Life of a Slave Girl” are preceded by a foreword by a white abolitionist simply because the white reading public could not accept that a former slave could write such a narrative on her own. I read Oliver Sacks’ introduction to Grandin’s “Emergence: Labeled Autistic” in the same light. For an academic to be perpetuating this crap is obscene, but unfortunately not surprising with regards to autistic people.

    I find Jureric’s idea of “autistic writing” rather odd in many ways. I’m officially diagnosed and I can generally write for clarity to an extent that exceeds most non-autistic people. I have been recognized as talented in this area since I was around six years old, which is when teachers started using my writing as an example to the other students. Despite having a lot of the typically autistic problems with socializing, I’ve always been very interested in and capable of constructing narratives which make sense. As a kid I often approached my life (and my imaginary lives) as written narratives to be constructed in my head. I still do this, actually. And I think one of the reasons my mother suspected that I might be on the spectrum originally was the fact that I can be so eloquent in written prose, yet often struggle to communicate verbally (unless I script).

    It’s not just Jureric’s classification of “autistic” writing that doesn’t make sense. It’s the assumption that non-autistic writing follows a clear logic, is “reader-centered,” etc. I’ve read a fair bit of undergraduate writing, having tutored undergraduates and now being a grader of undergraduate papers. I assume that most of the papers I’ve read are authored by neurotypical students. And yet problems with logic, transitions, audience awareness, and all of that are quite common–in spite of the fact that both institutions I’ve worked at are considered elite. But Jureric would have us believe that these issues are the exclusive domain of autistic people.

    That’s part of what I don’t like about making autism pathological: it obscures the common humanity between autistic and non-autistic people and makes it seem as though non-autistic people don’t struggle with writing and other tasks. Uh, no. That’s just wrong. Some autistic people are easily accessible writers; some are not. Same with non-autistic people. And all published writers rely on other people to help improve and clarify our work; that is the entire function of editors.

    Grandin’s published writings are apparently not legitimate examples, yet Jureric doesn’t extend the same standards to her own peer-reviewed academic article. Now that really doesn’t make sense.

    (Why Grandin as her only example, by the way? Though it’s true that some of Grandin’s work was co-authored, whatever that means, there are plenty of autistic writings that aren’t.)

    I might have to blog about the Jureric article now.

  6. Re: Attwood and typically-developing girls: I think it can happen sometimes, but it’s not always as benevolent as he would have us believe. Some autistic girls are the perfect victims of manipulation and control by a certain kind of typically developing girl. I know, having been through social ups and downs with a childhood “best friend” who was, in retrospect, an incredibly manipulative and controlling person. I generally thought she was wonderful at the time. I realize now that other girls were socially savvy enough to maintain some distance from her while I continually subjected myself to her manipulations and abuses in order to have a friend. Not fun.

  7. Thank you for a wonderful explanation/essay of a kind of life experience. I’ve only recently discovered that asperger’s is probably associated with some of the life difficulties I’ve had and been struggling to find some intellectual ‘ground’ to hold to.I am,however, very happy,in one sense, to know that neurology played a part, if only for its explanatory power.In particular that some of difficulties which I’ve caused others in my life haven’t been intentional but to some degree the result of ‘faulty wiring’, some evidence at least of an empathetic understanding of the pain felt by others? Thanks again, hope it passed……..

Comments are closed.